Skip to main content

Back to the starting blocks: language development study


Children’s toys and games at an early age – from baby ipads to other electronic gadgets – are increasingly designed to improve language development. A study published in December 2015 in the JAMA Pediatrics journal found that electronic gadgets may be no more than a distraction, and getting back to the starting blocks – the simple wooden alphabet blocks – and books, may still be the best toys for language development (Well, December 23, 2015).

Associate professor of communications science and disorders at Northern Arizona University in America, Anna V. Sosa, led a study on babies and electronic toys that were specifically advertised as language-promoters. The study is not new, as it buids on previous research into electronic toys and e-books and their impact on child development. However, the aim of the study was to capture ‘real world parent-child playtime in their homes without researchers watching.’

The study involved 26 families in America – most were white, female, and educated. Parents were given three sets of toys: (1) electronic toys including baby laptops, cellphones, and a talking farm, (2) traditional toys such as alphabet blocks and farm animal puzzles, and (3) board books about colours, shapes, and animals. Over three days, parents and their babies (aged 10 to 16 months) played for two 15-minute sessions with each set of toys. Parents were given audio recording devices that were turned on for the full three days, including the play sessions.

The results showed that when parents and babies played with electronic toys, that were designed as language-promoters, parents spoke less and responded less to baby babbling than when they played with traditional toys like blocks and books. Babies vocalized less when playing with electronic toys. The results were consistent for all families. The results were the same regardless of the gender of the baby, the age of the baby, the gender or age of the parent, or whether the parent was a ‘more chatty person’ or not.

When language-promoting electronic toys were used, parents said about 40 words per minute, on average, compared with 56 words per minute when they played with traditional blocks and 67 words per minute when they played with books.

Parents used fewer words that were relevant to the content of the toy. For example, when playing with books and blocks, parents were more likely to say ‘that’s a piggy’ or ‘that barn is red.’ Words and sentences, such as these reinforcer sentences, were said over four times more often with books than with electronic toys, and more than twice as often with traditional toys than with electronic toys.

Sosa said that it seemed as if parents were letting the baby interact with the electronic toy and therefore placing themselves on the sidelines. The electronic toys were providing some feedback – which the parents did for the traditional toys. Therefore parents let the electronic gadgets provide the feedback – do the talking – instead of the parents talking as much as they would during the play sessions with traditional toys and books.

Sosa said that for language-promoter toys and games, ideally, ‘a toy should be 10% toy and 90% child’ and added that ‘with a lot of these electronic toys, the toy takes over 90% and the child just fills in the blank.’

The researchers admitted that the sample size of just 26 families was a small study. Therefore there was little diversity of the families. A larger study would enable a more diversified group to be studied.

In addition, the study does not actually examine language development, per se – what it does is study the interaction between parent and baby during playtime with different sets of toys. It does not measure language development over a period of time. While the study aimed to be  a ‘real world’ view of playtime, it was still managed by the researchers – in that it was only for three days – therefore it didn’t study other interactions with other children and adults (as with brothers and sisters, carers, grandparents visiting the home etc.) or single parents versus non-single parents, and a range of other factors. In the real world, often there is a combination of toys during playtime – such as books, blocks, stuffed animals, and other objects around the home (spoons, pets, paper, parent’s fingers, etc.)

Image credit : krro.com



MARTINA NICOLLS is the author of:- The Shortness of Life: A Mongolian Lament (2015), Liberia’s Deadest Ends (2012), Bardot’s Comet (2011), Kashmir on a Knife-Edge (2010) and The Sudan Curse (2009).

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

Pir-E-Kamil - The Perfect Mentor by Umera Ahmed: book review

The Perfect Mentor pbuh  (2011) is set in Lahore and Islamabad in Pakistan. The novel commences with Imama Mubeen in medical university. She wants to be an eye specialist. Her parents have arranged for her to marry her first cousin Asjad. Salar Sikander, her neighbour, is 18 years old with an IQ of 150+ and a photographic memory. He has long hair tied in a ponytail. He imbibes alcohol, treats women disrespectfully and is generally a “weird chap” and a rude, belligerent teenager. In the past three years he has tried to commit suicide three times. He tries again. Imama and her brother, Waseem, answer the servant’s call to help Salar. They stop the bleeding from his wrist and save his life. Imama and Asjad have been engaged for three years, because she wants to finish her studies first. Imama is really delaying her marriage to Asjad because she loves Jalal Ansar. She proposes to him and he says yes. But he knows his parents won’t agree, nor will Imama’s parents. ...

Flaws in the Glass, a self-portrait by Patrick White: book review

The manuscript, Flaws in the Glass (1981), is Patrick Victor Martindale White’s autobiography. White, born in 1912 in England, migrated to Sydney, Australia, when he was six months old. For three years, at the age of 20, he studied French and German literature at King’s College at the University of Cambridge in England. Throughout his life, he published 12 novels. In 1957 he won the inaugural Miles Franklin Literary Award for Voss, published in 1956. In 1961, Riders in the Chariot became a best-seller, winning the Miles Franklin Literary Award. In 1973, he was the first Australian author to be awarded the Nobel Prize for Literature for The Eye of the Storm, despite many critics describing his works as ‘un-Australian’ and himself as ‘Australia’s most unreadable novelist.’ In 1979, The Twyborn Affair was short-listed for the Booker Prize, but he withdrew it from the competition to give younger writers the opportunity to win the award. His autobiography, Flaws in the Glass...

Sister cities discussed: Canberra and Islamabad

Two months ago, in March 2015, Australia and Pakistan agreed to explore ways to deepen ties. The relationship between Australia and Pakistan has been strong for decades, and the two countries continue to keep dialogues open. The annual bilateral discussions were held in Australia in March to continue engagements on a wide range of matters of mutual interest. The Pakistan delegation discussed points of interest will include sports, agriculture, economic growth, trade, border protection, business, and education. The possible twinning of the cities of Canberra, the capital of Australia, and Islamabad, the capital of Pakistan, were also on the agenda (i.e. called twin towns or sister cities). Sister City relationships are twinning arrangements that build friendships as well as government, business, culture, and community linkages. Canberra currently has international Sister City relationships with Beijing in China and Nara in Japan. One example of existing...